democracy in dialogue

naar de welkom pagina naar kinderen leren filosoferen naar ideeen voor de praktijk naar democratisch burgerschap naar delphi onderzoek naar rob bartels naar meer literatuur en websites  
home filosoferen kinderen democratie delphi rob more  
contact rob bartels edited: 04-05-2013  
   

In Dutch

         

Results of the first phase of the research

Aims and responsibilities

In March 2007 the programme and research project ‘Democracy in dialogue’ started in over 40 classes in primary schools. Both programme and research will run until summer 2008. The first phase ran from March to July 2007.

The programme ‘Philosophy for Democracy’ consists of four partial programs, each containing five elaborated themes:

‘A child can ask more than why’ for group 1/2, in which asking questions is the central focus.

‘All votes count’ for group 3/4. The main focus is the advancement of dialogue.

‘Just because isn’t a reason’ for group 5/6, in which the development of thinking skills is the central issue.

‘The big issue’ for group 7/8. In this part some democracy-related themes, such as freedom of speech, tolerance, etc. are elaborated.

The program ‘Philosophy for Democracy’ is an ‘ordinary’ program for philosophy with children. Apart from certain themes in group 7 and 8 there is no special emphasis on the development of democratic values or skills. The assumption is that philosophy with children is a democratic practice per se, and by using this practice, children develop democratic values and skills.

In the program special attention is paid to: 
- philosophical orientations for the teacher;
-
a large variety of scenarios to open up philosophical enquiries;
- the structure of the enquiry, which should focus on enabling elaboration and deep insight;
- closing activities.

The program should stimulate and enable teachers to philosophize at least once every two weeks with their children. The assumption is that when it is done less often, it is no longer effective.
The objective of the research is to show that philosophy with children can be a democratic practice which contributes to education for democratic citizenship: specifically, how all participants in a philosophical enquiry are equal and have an equal opportunity to influence the process; how an enquiry develops opinion, freedom of speech, and exchange; how philosophical enquiry shapes dialogue.
In the first phase of the research we wanted to know how children participate in philosophical enquiries as democratic practice. Do they indeed influence the process? Can and do they speak freely?

In this first phase we used three instruments:

a questionnaire for the teachers, consisting of three parts. The first part focused on information about the participating teachers: the class or age group they teach? What experience do they have in philosophy with children? What motivated them to join the program? The second part focused on their observations during the enquiries. Do children spontaneously give reasons for their opinions? Do they react rationally to differences of opinion? The third part reviewed the teachers’ assessment of the program. 24 out of 42 teachers completed the questionnaire

A short questionnaire for children from group 3 to 8. On this form we asked the children about their behaviour during the philosophy lessons. Questions like: Do you voice your own opinion? Can and do you speak freely and independently? 393 children completed the questionnaire

Class observations and interviews with eight teachers and their classes in order to clarify the statistical data from the questionnaire research. During classroom observation the same topics as on the questionnaire were used as a guideline.

The participants
Over forty teachers and the children in their classes joined the program in May 2007. Three complete school teams participated, the ‘Statenschool’ in Dordrecht, ’de Zevensprong’ in Boskoop, and ‘Icarus’ in Heemstede; nine teachers of the ‘Groningse Schoolvereniging’ and three teachers of primary school ‘Het Landje’ in Rotterdam participated as did six individual teachers. The research data are based on the response from Dordrecht, Boskoop, Heemstede, Rotterdam and from one individual teacher in Assendelft. They represent a fair amount of diversity, with regard to type of school (public, catholic, independent neutral), and to school population (inner city, multicultural, middle class).

The representation of different school years (from group 1/2 to group 7/8) is biased for teachers, as well as children. For the teachers, those with Group 5/6 or Group 7/8 are severely underrepresented, one of the main weaknesses of the research. With regard to the children, they range from group 3/4 to group 7/8 (there is no sense in giving the youngest group a written questionnaire), with overrepresentation of group 3/4. This is partly caused by the children of one school, ‘Het Landje’ in Rotterdam. In that school only the children in group 3/4 participated. Several comparisons between the schools were made. It was interesting to see that the practice of philosophizing, at least when considering the researched topics, did not vary much. The highest difference was 0,08 (on a five point scale)!.

Results

The teachers were very enthusiastic about the programme. The average assessment by the teachers was 4,06 on a five point scale. ‘This brings me so much structure’; ‘it just works’; the manuals make sure ‘that the enquiry doesn’t stay at the surface, it forces you to go deeper’.’ You have to work regularly with the program’, teachers said,’ then you will see the benefits’. Most of the teachers philosophized at least once in every two weeks. The ones who did it more frequently reported more progress. These teachers were probably the most enthusiastic ones. The program recommends that teachers and children philosophize once every two weeks. The enthusiasm of the teachers cannot only be explained by the quality of the program. The sole fact that it exists, was already seen as a great stimulus. 

Which behaviours do we observe during philosophizing with children?
Philosophizing starts with the asking of questions, by the children! That is at least what is assumed in the program guidelines. Starting questions focus the topic and direction of the dialogue, so the initial questioner has an important influence on the process in a philosophical enquiry. During the enquiries a lot of questions will be put forward, both by children and the teacher. The teachers ask questions, with which they also influence direction and the development of the enquiry.
During the enquiry children ask each other questions: why do you think so? Can you prove that? Does that mean …? Do you also think that …? Children are already able to do this when they are five years old. These are the questions that make dialogue out of a conversation. In this way the children’s questions shape the enquiry.
This is a remarkable feature. In the mainstream of daily school practice, there is barely room for children to ask and discuss questions. Countless research studies show how many questions teachers ask and the astonishing speed at which they do so (Dillon, 1982; Rowe, 1996; a.o.). Obviously it is hard for most teachers to drop this pattern while philosophizing with children. Perhaps the program manual contributes to this. In the manual teachers are shown a direction for the enquiry by means of questions which they have thought of in advance. This doesn’t stimulate leaving space for children to ask their own questions. The multiple asking of questions by the teachers threatens to make the teacher the central figure in a philosophical enquiry.

Every group has its ‘big mouths’ and ‘silent types’. The results show that teachers as well as children recognized this phenomenon. And all seemed to be a little unhappy with it. ‘At the beginning a lot of children speak. But then the enquiry is often taken over by familiar faces’, one teacher wrote. One of the children said: ‘a theme is interesting when everyone can talk about it, especially when everyone thinks differently about it.’ Letting everyone join in is a hard task in groups of over twenty children: ‘when I want to say something, then the others are still talking, they talk very fast one after another and then I don’t know what to say anymore’, sighed a girl. The teacher who gave turns, or went around the circle and let everyone join in, was appreciated as most democratic, but ‘not philosophical. The topic changes in between and you can’t react anymore’, said a boy who obviously was one of the ‘big mouths. The children thought that everyone had an equal opportunity to participate. In practice this was mainly realized by the use of duos and smaller groups during the enquiry.

The involvement of the children in philosophy was highly rated by the teachers, apart from the ones who teach group 1 or 2 (the youngest children). In written explanations and also in the observations and interviews with the teachers it became clear how difficult teachers found it to involve four-year-old children in the enquiry.

Interestingly, teachers also reported less involvement in group 7/8. This is also visible in the study with children, in answers to the question whether philosophy is about things they find interesting. However, when we interpret involvement as the degree to which children think about the questions that are discussed in philosophy we see high scores. Still, there was also a small change observable from group 5/6. It would be interesting to see if this picture fits with the more general picture of the appreciation by children of activities in school.
For our purposes, the answers on the topic ‘I always listen carefully to what other children say’ were very interesting. This topic had the highest average scores in the children’s research, and the spread of answers was not very broad. The score remained high in all groups. Thinking about what is being said and listening to what others say showed high scores from young to old.
Just as high, and for our purpose very interesting, were the scores with regard to autonomy: children answered themselves, their contribution to the enquiry was authentic and they contributed independently from others. The high average scores and the low standard deviations (for the teachers of group 3/4) show that the picture was consistent. Only with the youngest (group 1/2) was this score a little lower.

Children give reasons for their opinions, and they do so more and more spontaneously as they get older, is the conclusion. ‘They develop as they get older’, wrote one of the teachers of group 3/4. This is confirmed when we see the results of the children’s research. That younger children (group 1/2) found it hard to give reasons`(mainly the four year olds, according to the teachers) is not surprising.

Can children deal reasonably with objections and differences of opinion? In all foregoing topics we saw that teachers and children did not rate the practice of philosophy very differently. The trend in their answers was always the same. Not on this particular item, however. The teachers may have too rosy a picture of how children feel about objections and differences of opinion. They seemed to agree on this: yes, children react rationally to objections. But the children themselves said, “No, it is annoying when others don’t agree with you’. As children got older this diminished. Maybe this is a growing process. Of course, we must take into account that teachers and children responded to a different formulation of the question. The teachers were asked about observable behaviour, while the children were asked about their feelings. In a way this makes it even more interesting from the perspective of democratic citizenship development: although children do not like to, they still act rationally when differences of opinion arise.

Philosophy with children is a democratic practice!

The results form the first phase of the research seem to confirm our main hypothesis: Philosophy with children is a democratic practice!

Although the teacher is the dominant source of questions within the philosophical enquiry, and because of this, decisive for its direction and development, we also observe considerable influence from the children. At a young age, they already have dialogical attitude and skills, such as asking one another questions, listening to one another, and these attitudes and skills develop more and more as they get older.

Everyone has equal opportunities to participate. The enquiry within the full circle of an entire class of children is mostly dominated by some ‘big mouths, but by using smaller circles and groups everyone still gets their turn. The involvement of children in the enquiries is high.

The opinions expressed are of the children themselves, their contribution to the enquiry is authentic and they contribute independently from one another. The four- and five-year-olds sometimes watch each other first, but from group 3 (age 6) upwards they are admirably involved. This even develops further.

Children give reasons for their opinions, and they do so more and more spontaneously as they get older. It is hard to think of new reasons, when you have to defend your point of view. It is even annoying when someone else disagrees with you. Obviously that does not feel comfortable, but in their reactions that discomfort - as children get older – is less and less observable.